COAS
Center for Open Access in Science (COAS)
OPEN JOURNAL FOR STUDIES IN PHILOSOPHY (OJSP)

ISSN (Online) 2560-5380 * ojsp@centerprode.com

OJSP Home

2022 - Volume 6 - Number 1


The Language Origin Paradigms: Conceptual Typologies, Results Accumulation, and Theoretical Perspectives

Nikolai S. Rozov * nrozov@gmail.com * ORCID: 0000-0003-2362-541X
Novosibirsk State Technical University, Department of International Relations and Regional Studies, Novosibirsk, RUSSIAN FEDERATION

Open Journal for Studies in Philosophy, 2022, 6(1), 21-38 * https://doi.org/10.32591/coas.ojsp.0601.03021r
Received: 16 June 2022 ▪ Revised: 18 August 2022 ▪ Accepted: 25 August 2022

LICENCE: Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

ARTICLE (Full Text - PDF)


ABSTRACT:
Studies of the origin and early evolution of language are undergoing rapid development. New directions, topics and methods of research are appearing. It becomes necessary to structure the research field, as the formation of a single space of intellectual attention contributes to a more productive communication between different groups of scientists, better mutual understanding, and strengthened arguments. The article proposes the grounds for typologizing the concepts of the origin of language and identifies three main paradigms on the basis of these typologies. The sharpest disputes are between Continualism (“human language is only quantitatively different from animal language”) and Saltationism (“human language is so fundamentally different from the communicative systems of animals, that it could only appear as a result of some amazing leap or unique mutation”). Continualists usually focus on the communicative and cognitive abilities of different species (with a clear preference for apes). Saltationists focus on the linguistic, mental and cognitive structures of humans. Both of these polar approaches are opposed by the Multistage ecosocial paradigm, which emphasizes a long, multistep process of glottogenesis with behavioral adaptations of hominid groups to the changing natural and social conditions of their existence. A list of the most plausible, theoretically substantiated propositions is given, as well as research results supported by a variety of circumstantial data. The theoretical and methodological perspectives of the multistage ecosocial paradigm related to overcoming from its drawbacks are presented. A generalizing conceptual framework corresponding to the basic principles of evolution, the laws of social interaction should be constructed. The extremely heterogeneous arsenal of methods should also be structured in a single scientific logic.

KEY WORDS: language origin, glottogenesis, stages of language evolution, cognitive evolution, typologies of conceptions, research programs, nomological approach, cultural drive, gene-cultural coevolution.

CORRESPONDING AUTHOR:
Nikolai S. Rozov, Novosibirsk State Technical University, Department of International Relations and Regional Studies, Novosibirsk, RUSSIAN FEDERATION. E-mail: nrozov@gmail.com.


 

REFERENCES:

Alexander, R. D. (1990). How did humans evolve? Reflections on the uniquely unique species. University of Michigan Museum of Zoology, Special Publications, 1, 1-38.

Arbib, M. A. 2005. From monkey-like action recognition to human language: an evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences28(2), 105-124.

Arbib, M. A. (2017). Toward the language-ready brain: Biological evolution and primate comparisons. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 24, 142-150.

Belyaev, D. K. (1979). Destabilizing selection as a factor in domestication. The J. of Heredity, 70(5), 301-308.

Bernabeu, P., Vogt P. (2015). Language evolution: Current status and future directions. In 10th LangUE Conference (pp. 1-27). Essex, UK.

Bickerton, D. (1981). Roots of language. Language Science Press.

Bickerton, D. (2009). Adam’s tongue: How humans made language, how language made humans. New York: Hill and Wang.

Bingham, P. (2010). On the evolution of language: implications of a new and general theory of human origins, properties, and history. In R. K. Larson, V. Déprez & H. Yamakido (Eds.), The evolution of human language: Biolinguistic perspectives (pp. 211-224). Cambridge Univ. Press.

Boer, B. de (2011). Loss of air sacs improved hominin speech abilities. J. of Human Evolution, 62(1), 1-6.

Boer, B. de (2017). Evolution of speech and evolution of language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, vol. 24, pp. 158–162.

Boer, B. de, Thompson, B., Ravignani, A., & Boeckx, C. (2020). Evolutionary dynamics do not motivate a single-mutant theory of human language. Scientific Reports10(1), 451.

Bouchard, D. (2013). The nature and origin of language. Oxford Univ. Press.

Breyl, M. (2021). Triangulating Neanderthal cognition: A tale of not seeing the forest for the trees. WIREs Cogn Sci; e1545. URL: https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1545

Burling, R. (2005). The talking ape. How language evolved. Oxford Univ. Press.

Bybee, J. (2002). Sequentiality as the basis of constituent structure. In T. Givón & B. F. Malle (Eds.), The evolution of language out of pre‑language (pp. 109-134). Amsterdam; Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

Byrne, R. W. (1996). Machiavellian intelligence. Evolutionary Anthropology, 5(5), 172-180.

Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1997). Genes, peoples, and languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94, 7719-7724.

Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin, and use. Westport, Connecticut: Praeger.

Chomsky, N. (2016). The language capacity: Architecture and evolution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 01 July 2016.

Christiansen, M., & Kirby, S. (2003). Language evolution: Consensus and controversies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 300-307.

Collins, R. (1998). The sociology of philosophies. A global theory of intellectual change. Harvard Univ. Press.

Collins, R. (2004). Interaction ritual chains. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press.

Corballis, M. C. (2010). Did language evolve before speech? In R. K. Larson, Déprez, V., & H. Yamakido (Eds.), The evolution of human language: Biolinguistic perspectives (pp. 115-123). Cambridge Univ. Press.

Darwin, Ch. (1871/1981). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. Princeton Univ. Press.

Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species: The coevolution of language and the brain. New York, NY: Norton.

Dediu, D. et al. (2013). Cultural evolution of language. In P. J. Richerson & M. H. Christiansen (Eds.), Cultural evolution: Society, technology, language, and religion (pp. 303-332). MIT Press.

Dessalles, J.-L. (2007). Why we talk. The evolutionary origins of language. Oxford Univ. Press.

Donald, M. (1998). Hominid enculturation and cognitive evolution. In C. Scarre (Ed.), Cognition and material culture: The archaeology of symbolic storage (pp. 7-17). Cambridge: McDonald Institute.

Donald, M. (2001). A mind so rare: The evolution of human consciousness. New York: Norton.

Donald, M. (2017). Key cognitive preconditions for the evolution of language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 204-208.

Dor, D. (2015). The instruction of imagination: Language as a social communication technology. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.

Dor, D., Knight, Chr., & Lewis, J. (Eds.) (2014). The social origins of language. Oxford Univ. Press.

Dunbar, R. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press.

Dunbar, R. (2003). The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 163-181.

Enard, W., Przeworski, M.., Fisher, M. S., Lai, L., Wiebe, C. S. L., Takashi Kitano, V., Monaco, P., & Paabo, S. (2002). Molecular revolution of FOXP2, A gene involved in speech and language. Nature, 418, 869-872.

Engels, F. (1884/2010). The origin of the family, private property, and the state. Penguin Classics.

Nolfi, S., & Mirolli, M. (Eds.) (2010). Evolution of communication and language in embodied agents. Springer.

Falk, D. (2004). Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: Whence motherese? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 491-541.

Falk, D. (2016). Evolution of brain and culture. J. of Anthropological Sciences, 94(1), 99-111.

Fitch, W. T. (2010). The evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Fitch, W. T. (2017). Empirical approaches to the study of language evolution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, February, 1, 1-31.

Gabora, L., & Smith, C. M. (2018). Two cognitive transitions underlying the capacity for cultural evolution. J. of Anthropological Sciences, 96, 1-26.

Gärdenfors, P., & Osvath, M. (2010). Prospection as a cognitive precursor to symbolic communication. In R. K. Larson, V. Déprez & H. Yamakido (Eds.), The evolution of human language: Biolinguistic perspectives (pp. 103-114). Cambridge Univ. Press.

Givón, T. (2009). The genesis of syntactic complexity: Diachrony, ontogeny, neuro‑cognition, evolution. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publ. Company.

Gould, S. J., & Eldridge, N. (1977). Punctuated equilibria: The tempo and mode of evolution reconsidered. Paleobiology, 3, 115-151.

Hare, B., Wobber, V., & Wrangham, R. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: Evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Animal Behaviour, 83, 573-585.

Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002).  The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science,22, 298(5598), 1569-1579.

Henrich, J. (2015). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.

Henshilwood, Chr. S., & Dubreuil, B. (2011). The Still Bay and Howiesons Poort, 77–59 ka symbolic material culture and the evolution of the mind during the African Middle Stone Age. Current Anthropology, 52(3), 361-400.

Heyes, C. M. (2012). What can imitation do for cooperation? In B. Calcott, R. Joyce & K. Sterelny (Eds.), Signaling, Commitment & Cooperation (pp. 313-331). MIT Press.

Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother nature: A history of mothers, infants, and natural selection. Boston: Pantheon Books.

Hurford, J. R (2012). Language in the light of evolution: Vol. II, The origins of grammar. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.

Iriki, A. (2005). A prototype of Homo faber: A silent precursor of human intelligence in the tool-using monkey brain. In S. Dehaene, J.-R. Duhamel, M. D. Hauser & G. Rizzolatti (Eds.), From Monkey Brain to Human Brain: A Fyssen Foundation Symposium (pp. 253-271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Irvine, L., Roberts, S., & Kirby, S. (2013). A robustness approach to theory building: A case study of language evolution. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 35(35).

Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford Univ. Press.

Kay, R., Cartmill, M., & Balow, M. (1998). The hypoglossal canal and the origin of human vocal behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(9), 5417-5419.

Kirby, S. (2017). Culture and biology in the origins of linguistic structure. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 118-137.

Knight, Chr. (2006). Language co-evolved with the rule of law. In A. Cangelosi, A. D. M. Smith & K. Smith (Eds.). Evolution of language. Proceedings…6th International Conference (EVOLANG6) (pp. 168-175). 12-15 April 2006, Rome, Italy.

Lakatos, I. (1978). The methodology of scientific research programmes.In J. Worrall & G. Currie (Eds.) Philosophical papers,1, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Laland K. N. (2017). Darwin’s unfinished symphony. How culture made the human mind. Princeton & Oxford: Princeton Univ. Press.

Lloyd, E. A. (2004). Kanzi, evolution, and language. Biology and Philosophy, 19, 577-588.

Lovejoy, C. O. (2009). Reexamining human origins in light of Ardipithecus ramidus. Science, 326(5949), 74–74e8 

Luria, A. R. (1981). Language and cognition. Wiley.

Maclarnon, A., & Hewitt G. (2004). Increased breathing control: Another factor in the evolution of human language. Evolutionary Anthropology, 13, 181-197.

Markov, A. V., & Markov M. A. (2020). Runaway brain-culture coevolution as a reason for larger brains: Exploring the “cultural drive” hypothesis by computer modeling. Ecology and evolution, 10(12), 1-19.

Martínez et al., (2004) — Martínez I., Rosa M., Arsuaga J.-L., Jarabo P., Quam R., Lorenzo C., Gracia A., Carretero J.-M., Bermudez de Castro J.-M., & Carbonell E. Auditory capacities in Middle Pleistocene humans from the Sierra de Atapuerca in Spain. Proc. National Academy of Science (PNAS), 101(27), 9976-9981.

Miller, G. F. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday.

Morgan et al. (2015) — Morgan, T. J. H., Uomini, N. T., Rendell, L. E., Chouinard-Thuly, L., Street, S. E., Lewis, H. M., Cross, C. P., Evans, C., Kearney, R., de la Torre, I., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. Experimental evidence for the coevolution of hominin tool-making, teaching and language. Nature Communications, 6(6029), 1-8.

Page, A. E., & French, J. C. (2020). Reconstructing prehistoric demography: What role for extant hunter-gatherers? Evolutionary Anthropology, 29(6), 332-345.

Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York, NY: William Morrow and Company.

Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA (PNAS), 107, 8993-8999.

Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324, 1298-1301.

Power, C. (2014). Signal evolution and the social brain. In D. Dor, C. Knight & J. Lewis (Eds.). The social origins of language (pp. 47-55). Oxford Univ. Press.

Richerson, P. J.Boyd, R. (1992). Cultural inheritance and evolutionary ecology. In E. A. Smith & B. Winterhalder (Eds.), Evolutionary ecology and human behavior (pp. 61-92). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Univ. of Chicago Press.

Richerson, P., Boyd, R., & Bettinger, R. (2009). Cultural innovations and demographic change. Human Biology, 81, 211-235.

Roberts, S. G. et al. (2020). CHIELD: the causal hypotheses in evolutionary linguistics database. J. of Language Evolution,5(2), 101-120.

Rozov, N. S. (2022). The origins of language and consciousness: How social orders and communicative concerns gave rise to speech and cognitive abilities. Novosibirsk: Manuscript (in Russian).

Rumbaugh, M. D. (2013). With apes in mind: Emergents, communication & competence. CreateSpace.

Rumbaugh, M. D. (2015). A salience theory of learning and behavior and rights of apes. In J. H. Turner, R. Machalek & A. Maryanski (Eds.), Handbook on evolution and society: Toward an evolutionary social science (pp. 514-536). New York, NY: Routledge/Paradigm.

Saussure, F. D. (1986). The linguistic sign. In R. E. Innis (Ed.), Semiotics. An introductory anthology (pp. 28-46). London: Hutchinson.
Sterelny, K. (2016). Cumulative cultural evolution and the origins of language. Biological Theory, 25 July, 173-186.

Stout, D. (2002). Skill and cognition in stone tool production: An ethnographic case study from Irian Jaya. Current Anthropology, 43(5), 693-722.

Stout, D. (2005). The social and cultural context of stone-knapping skill acquisition. In V. Roux & B. Bril (Eds.), Stone knapping: The necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour (pp. 331-340). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

Stringer, Chr. (2012). Lone survivors: How we came to be the only humans on earth. Times Books.

Tamariz, M., & Kirby, S. (2016). The cultural evolution of language. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 37-43.

Tomasello, M. (2008). The origins of human communication. Massachusetts, MA: MIT Press.

Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming human a theory of ontogeny. Harvard Univ. Press.

Turner, J. H., & Machalek, R. (2018). The new evolutionary sociology: Recent and revitalized theoretical and methodological approaches. New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.

Turner, J. H., & Maryanski, A. (2008). On the origin of societies by natural selection. Boulder, CO: Paradigm Publishers.

White et al., (2015) — White, T. D., Lovejoy, C. O., Asfaw, B., Carlson, J. P., & Suwa, G. Neither chimpanzee nor human, Ardipithecus reveals the surprising ancestry of both. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA (PNAS)112(16), 4877-4884.

Wildgen, W. (2012). Language evolution as a cascade of behavioral bifurcations. Estudios de. Linguistica Universidad de Alicante, 26, 359-382.

Wilson, E., & Lumsden, Ch. J. (1983). Promethean fire. Reflections on the origin of the mind. Harvard Univ. Press.

Wood, B., & Bauernfeind, A. (2012). The fossil record: Evidence for speech in early hominins. In M. Tallerman & K. Gibson (Eds.), Language evolution (pp. 258-272). Oxford Univ. Press.

Wrangham, R. (2010). Catching fire. How cooking made us human. Profile Books.

Wrangham, R. (2019). The goodness paradox: The strange relationship between virtue and violence in human evolution. Pantheon.

Zlatev, J. (2014). The coevolution of human intersubjectivity, morality, and language. In D. Dor, C. Knight & J. Lewis (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 249-266). Oxford Univ. Press.

 

© Center for Open Access in Science